Main

Inequalities in cancer survival may be explained by differences in tumour biology, access to effective treatment and stage at diagnosis. Delay in presentation with symptoms among patients with cancer is likely to contribute to late stage at diagnosis and, thereby, poorer survival (Richards et al, 1999; Neal, 2009; De Angelis et al, 2014).

How long a cancer patient takes to present may be influenced by several factors, including the nature of the symptoms, awareness of the significance of the symptoms, perception of personal risk of cancer, and physical, social and psychological barriers to health care (Ramirez et al, 1999; Macleod et al, 2009). In the UK, awareness in the general population that the risk of cancer increases with age is low and reporting that embarrassment and worry about wasting the doctor’s time might put them off going to the doctor is common compared with similar countries (Forbes et al, 2013). However, we do not yet have evidence to explain fully why some cancer patients delay symptomatic presentation. This evidence is important to inform interventions to promote early presentation (Austoker et al, 2009).

Approaches to promoting early presentation generally focus on specific cancers (Austoker et al, 2009; Athey et al, 2011; Forbes et al, 2011). However, when a patient experiences symptoms, they may not associate these with the possibility of a specific cancer and seek help, even if they have been exposed to a campaign – particularly if their symptoms are not specific for cancer, for example, weight loss or abdominal pain. Campaigns to promote early symptomatic presentation of cancer may have more impact on mortality if they address the risk factors for delay in presentation in general.

We aimed to identify risk factors for the delay in presentation by socio-demographic characteristics, cancer type, symptom type and reported reasons for not going to the doctor across a population of cancer patients, to inform the development of public health approaches to promote early presentation.

Materials and Methods

Sampling

Cancer patients were selected from people who had taken part in the English Department of Health’s Cancer Patient Experience Survey in mid-2010 (Department of Health, 2010). This included all adult cancer patients who had received treatment at 1 of 158 National Health Service (NHS) hospitals in England from January-March 2010. These patients had been sent a postal questionnaire asking about their experiences of health care, and 67 713 patients responded (66%).

Of the respondents, 53 104 agreed that they could be recontacted for further surveys. We identified who among these had died using the national Demographic Batch Service system (Health and Social Care Information Centre, 2014), which maintains an up-to-date list of all NHS patients. We randomly sampled 200 patients, each with 1 of 22 cancer types (Table 1 provides International Classification of Diseases codes), from those alive in November 2011. Only 181 patients with pancreatic cancer were alive in November 2011, so we sent a questionnaire to all of them. The cancer types were selected to include common cancers and some rarer cancers for which we had identified a lack of information about nature and duration of symptoms. We aimed for a sample size of 3000 for patients with 1 of all 22 cancer types, which would provide reasonable precision around estimates of frequency of symptoms for the entire population of cancer patients.

Table 1 Proportions of patients alive in November 2011, of those who took part in the National Cancer Patient Experience Survey 2010, and response to the survey among those sampled

For the analysis described in this paper, we included only patients with 1 of 15 cancers where >75% patients who had taken part in the Cancer Patient Experience Survey 2010 were still alive in November 2011 (Table 1). This was to minimise ‘survivor’ bias introduced by surveying only those who were alive at least 16 months after the Cancer Patient Experience survey. Cancers included in the analysis were cancers of breast, colon, rectum, cervix, endometrium, oral cavity, kidney/ureter, bladder and prostate; malignant melanoma; sarcoma; chronic myeloid leukaemia, chronic lymphocytic leukaemia, non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma, and myeloma. These 15 cancer types represent 61% of the population of cancer patients in England (excluding non-melanoma skin cancer) (Office for National Statistics, 2011). The most common cancers that we excluded from the analysis were lung and upper gastrointestinal cancers.

The questionnaire

The patients were sent a self-complete questionnaire asking about the nature of the symptoms that had caused them to see a doctor and led to diagnosis. Patients were asked to identify symptoms from a list, because this type of data could be analysed more reliably than responses to open questions. Participants could tick more than one symptom. We developed cancer-specific versions of the questionnaire (shown in Supplementary Material), so as to keep the list of symptoms short. The symptom lists were developed with the input of clinical experts in the relevant cancers.

The questionnaire asked participants how long it was from noticing the symptom that led to diagnosis to presenting to a doctor (<2 weeks/2–4 weeks/>4 weeks to 3 months/>3 months to 6 months/>6 months to 12 months/>1 year), the date the presenting symptom started and date of first going to a doctor. The questionnaire asked what had put the patient off seeing a doctor (based on those in the validated Awareness and Beliefs about Cancer measure (Simon et al, 2012) and Cancer Research UK Cancer Awareness Measure (Stubbings et al, 2009)).

Questionnaires for breast, bowel and cervical cancer also asked about whether the patient had been diagnosed via a NHS Cancer Screening Programme.

The questionnaire was based on our previous studies of breast and cervical cancer (Burgess et al, 1998; Lim et al, 2013). The questionnaires were piloted and cognitively tested (Campanelli, 1997) in three rounds. This aimed to ensure that the questions were easy to complete, acceptable and understandable, and to test the ability of patients to recall events. The patients, recruited through Macmillan Cancer Support, were first sent the questionnaires. Trained interviewers then conducted telephone interviews, asking the patients to describe in their own words what they thought the questions were asking, and ‘think aloud’ to describe how they would answer.

Data collection

A postal survey was carried out between November 2011 and January 2012. We sent two reminders to non-responders.

Statistical analysis

We excluded all patients who reported being diagnosed via a screening programme (i.e., breast, bowel, cervix) or who reported no symptoms. We assigned each patient an Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) based on postcode (Public Health Observatory, 2011). The IMD provides a measure of socioeconomic deprivation based on the characteristics of the area of residence of each individual (Department for Communities and Local Government, 2014), a higher score indicating greater deprivation. We categorised people into quintiles of IMD in England.

We categorised symptoms into nine categories (or ‘other) (Supplementary Box S1, online supplement), to reflect a patient’s experience rather than the pathology underlying the symptoms. We coded free text symptoms into these categories where possible.

We weighted the percentages reporting each symptom for the incidence of the cancer (using 2009 national cancer registration data (Office for National Statistics, 2011) to give percentages more representative of the whole population of cancer patients.

We calculated duration of symptoms leading to diagnosis based on the response to the question: ‘In these first few questions we would like to know about the MAIN health problems or symptoms that made you go to see the GP (family doctor), or made you go to accident and emergency, and that led to you being diagnosed with cancer…. How long was it from the time you noticed these MAIN health problems or symptoms and first seeing a GP or hospital doctor?’ (Questionnaires provided in Supplementary Material). Where patients had not responded to the question about duration of symptoms, we calculated duration from dates provided by the patient, if they had given day, month and year for both onset of symptoms and date of seeing a doctor. We considered >3 months from onset of symptoms to presentation as a ‘delay’, as this is conventional and likely to represent a clinically important delay (Richards et al, 1999).

We calculated relative risks for delay in presentation, fitting log-binomial regression models using the Stata command glm, by cancer, presenting symptom and reasons for putting off going to the doctor. We controlled for age, sex and deprivation group, because we judged in advance that these might influence time to presentation (Ramirez et al, 1999; Macleod et al, 2009).

As a survey of perceptions of NHS patient care designed to improve services, no formal ethics approval was sought for the Cancer Patient Experience Survey by the Department of Health. However, all patients approached, as part of our survey had given written consent to being recontacted.

Results

Completeness of sample

In late 2011, the proportion of those who took part in the Cancer Patient Experience Survey 2010 who were alive at the time of our survey varied by cancer (Table 1), from 92% in malignant melanoma to 77% for cancer of the oral cavity.

Response

We sent questionnaires to 4381 patients. These included 3000 patients with 1 of the 15 cancer types selected for this analysis. We received completed questionnaires from 2371 (79%) of these patients. Response varied by cancer, from 67% (cervix cancer) to 88% (rectal cancer) (Table 1).

Participants

In total, 1999 patients reported any symptoms and that they had not been diagnosed via an NHS Cancer Screening Programme. All had been diagnosed in the year before the Cancer Patient Experience Survey 2010. Of the patients reporting symptoms, 1077 (54%) were male, 534 (27%) were aged 70+ and 1539 (77%) were aged 50+; all were of White ethnic group. In the population of cancer patients registered in England in 2009 with 1 of the 15 cancer types, 51% were male, 47% were aged 70+ and 89% were aged 50+ (Office for National Statistics, 2011). Median IMD was 13.8 (English median is 15) (Public Health Observatory, 2011).

What were the symptoms that led to a diagnosis of cancer?

Table 2 shows the proportions of patients with each cancer type reporting each type of symptom. There were relatively few women with breast or cervical cancer because many of these reported that they had been diagnosed by NHS Cancer Screening Programmes.

Table 2 Symptom types reported by 1999 patients reporting at least one symptom, by diagnosis

Table 2 also shows the frequency of symptoms overall and by cancer. The five commonest presenting symptoms were pain (reported by 29%), lump (28%), bleeding (23%), difficulty with urination (18%) and systemic symptoms (17%). Among the patients with pain, musculoskeletal pain and abdominal pain were equally common (each reported by 9% of the total). Rectal bleeding was the commonest type of bleeding (10%) followed by urinary (8%) and vaginal bleeding (4%). The commonest systemic symptom was fatigue (13%). Eating-related symptoms and chest symptoms were uncommon, which is likely to be because we excluded patients with upper gastrointestinal and lung cancers.

What put you off going to the doctor?

In total, 965 patients (48%) reported anything that put them off going to the doctor (Table 3), most commonly not realising that the symptom was serious (27%). Most patients reporting this had common presenting symptoms: 33% pain, 29% systemic symptoms, 22% lump or swelling, and 21% bleeding. Other reasons for putting off going to the doctor were rarely reported, the most common being worry about wasting the doctor’s time, reported by 6%.

Table 3 Frequency of reported reasons for putting off going to the doctor (1999 patients reporting at least one symptom)

How long was it from noticing the symptoms to first seeing a doctor?

We had data on duration of symptoms for 1835 patients (92%). The proportion with these data available varied by cancer, being most complete in non-Hodgkin’s lymphoma (96%) and least complete in chronic leukaemia (86%) (Supplementary Table S1, Supplementary Material). There was no difference in distribution of sex and IMD between those with and without duration of symptoms data, although data completeness varied slightly by age, being more complete in younger people (aged 70+: 90%; aged 50–69: 94%; aged <50: 95%, P=<0.01).

In total, 423 (21%) patients reported delay in presentation (>3 months from first noticing the symptom to first going to see a doctor).

Risk factors for delay in presentation

Age, sex and socioeconomic status

There was no difference in frequency of delay by age or sex, although there was a trend suggesting that older people might be less likely to delay (Table 4). The most socioeconomically deprived group was most likely to report delay (29%).

Table 4 Demographic risk factors for delay in presentation (1835 patients with at least one symptom and known duration of symptoms)

Cancer type

Patients with prostate (48%) and rectal cancer (37%) were most likely to report delay, and were statistically significantly more likely to report delay than patients with chronic lymphocytic leukaemia (chosen as the reference group because the proportion reporting delay was closest to the pooled average) (Table 5). Eight percent of women with breast cancer reported delay and this was the only group statistically significantly less likely to report delay than patients with chronic lymphocytic leukaemia.

Table 5 Risk factors for delay in symptomatic presentation: cancer type, symptom and reasons for putting off going to the doctor

Symptom type

The symptoms statistically significantly associated with delay in presentation were difficulties with urination (41%), change of bowel habit (38%), skin symptoms (31%), and systemic symptoms (29%). There was an association of borderline statistical significance between pain and delay in presentation (26%). Patients reporting lump or swelling were statistically significantly less likely to have delayed than patients not reporting lump or swelling (Table 5).

Symptom type was strongly associated with cancer type. Most patients with urinary difficulties had either prostate or bladder cancer (84%). Most patients with change of bowel habit (86%) had cancer of the colon or rectum. Most patients with skin symptoms (91%) had malignant melanoma or haemato-oncological cancer. This means that it was not possible to identify whether the symptom type itself, or some other feature of the cancer, was causally associated with delay.

Patients with abdominal pain were slightly more likely to report delay (31%) than those with musculoskeletal pain (25%). Although bleeding overall was not associated with delay in presentation, the type of bleeding did appear to be important: 35% of patients with rectal bleeding, 30% of patients with vaginal bleeding, but only 9% of patients with urinary bleeding delayed presentation.

Reported reasons for putting off going to the doctor

Patients who reported that they had not realised that the symptom was serious were more likely to delay presentation than patients who did not report this (Table 5). Being too embarrassed to see the doctor, worry about what the doctor might find, worry about wasting the doctor’s time and being too busy were all statistically significantly associated with delay in presentation (Table 5). Finding it difficult to make an appointment was not associated with delay in presentation.

Discussion

Delay in symptomatic presentation for >3 months in this group of cancer patients was common. Delay was more common in people of lower socioeconomic status. Strong and common risk factors for delay were prostate cancer, rectal cancer, urinary symptoms, change of bowel habit, rectal bleeding, systemic symptoms, skin symptoms and reporting not having recognised the symptoms as serious.

This survey is unique, as far as we know, in attempting to quantify symptoms and risk factors for delay in presentation in a large population of patients with a range of cancers. The design and reporting were consistent with the Aarhus Statement on early cancer diagnosis research (Weller et al, 2012).

We excluded some common cancers from the analysis, including lung, pancreas, stomach and oesophagus. This was because many of the patients with these cancers had died between diagnosis and the survey. Those who were alive would have been an unusual group; including these would have introduced a greater degree of survivor bias and made our results less generalisable to the whole population of cancer patients. An important limitation of our study is that the conclusions are generalisable to patients with the cancers we have included (who represent about 60% of cancer patients in the UK) and those who survive for at least a year after diagnosis.

Older people were underrepresented in our study, perhaps because they have worse cancer survival (De Angelis et al, 2014) and are more likely to have died between the Cancer Patient Experience Survey 2010 and our survey. We cannot, therefore, be sure that the findings of no association between age and delay in presentation are reliable. People of non-White ethnic origin were also underrepresented; all patients in our survey were of White ethnic origin. This is at least partly because non-White people were underrepresented in the Cancer Patient Experience Survey 2010 (4% were of non-White origin (Department of Health, 2010), whereas about 13% of the adult population of England are of non-White origin (Office for National Statistics, 2013)). We cannot be sure that our findings would apply to patients of non-White ethnic group.

Patients may not be able to recall nature and duration of symptoms, and their reasons for not going to the doctor, accurately and precisely (Jenkins et al, 2002). Recall error may have caused bias in our study if people who delayed presentation were more likely to report reasons for putting off going to the doctor, perhaps seeking to account for their delay. If this was the case, however, we might have expected people who delayed presentation to be more likely to report difficulty in making an appointment as well as, for example, embarrassment or worry about wasting the doctor’s time.

We found some evidence that lower socioeconomic position was a risk factor for delay in presentation. The existing evidence on the association between low socioeconomic position and delayed presentation of cancer is conflicting: low socioeconomic position has been found to be a risk factor for delay in presentation of prostate cancer, but not for delay in presentation of colorectal cancer or gynaecological cancer (Macleod et al, 2009). This is important because people of low socioeconomic position have more advanced cancer at diagnosis and worse survival (Rachet et al, 2010; Lyratzopoulos et al, 2012); why this is not clear and it may be at least partly due to longer time to presentation.

Our study has some similar findings to a recently published study documenting GPs’ reports of cancer patients’ delays in presentation (Keeble et al, 2014). The definition of a ‘non-prompt’ presentation in that study was duration of symptoms of 15 or more days; overall about half the patients delayed presentation. As in our study, delay was less likely in patients with breast or bladder cancer and more likely in patients with colorectal cancer. However, in contrast, in that study, prostate cancer was not associated with delay, and oropharyngeal cancer was associated with delay. Whether these contrasting results are due to survival bias in our sample or differences in methods of collecting data on duration of symptoms is not clear.

A US study also found, as we did, that patients with prostate cancer were more likely to delay presentation than patients with breast or colorectal cancer (Samet et al, 1988). Most patients with prostate cancer in our study reported urinary difficulties (and vice versa). Urinary difficulties are common in older men with benign prostatic disease (Kupelian et al, 2006). Although urinary symptoms may have led to the investigation and diagnosis of prostate cancer, they may be due to coexisting benign conditions rather than the cancer.

Rectal cancer has been found in previous studies to be associated with longer duration of symptoms than colon cancer (Mitchell et al, 2008). Change in bowel habit, in our study, was almost exclusively reported by bowel cancer patients, and was associated with delay in presentation. Change in bowel habit has been reported to be a risk factor for delay in presentation in another study (Mor et al, 1990) but another found that it was not a risk factor (Pedersen et al, 2013). As with urinary difficulties, change in bowel habit may have an insidious, rather than a dramatic onset, and for short periods of time at least, may be regarded as part of normal variation in bodily functions. Similar issues may explain delay in presentation among patients with skin symptoms, pain and systemic symptoms, particularly fatigue, which is very common in the general population (Pawlikowska et al, 1994; Chou, 2013). Other studies have also suggested that non-specific symptoms are associated with patient delay (Macleod et al, 2009). The evidence relating to pain as a risk factor for patient delay varies by cancer: it is associated with greater delay in urological cancers, but less delay in gastrointestinal cancers (Mitchell et al, 2008; Macleod et al, 2009).

Most patients with rectal cancer reported bleeding, most commonly rectal bleeding, and this was associated with longer duration of symptoms, a finding that echoes other studies (Mor et al, 1990; Pedersen et al, 2013). This is surprising, as it is a dramatic symptom, and is likely to be due to pathology. However, we hypothesise that people assume that their bleeding is caused by haemorrhoids, which affect 4–5% of the adult population (Johanson and Sonnenberg, 1990).

Delay in presentation in breast cancer has fallen over the last 15 years: a systematic review in 1999 patients reported that 18–29% of breast cancer patients delayed presentation for >3 months and reported a decade upon decade fall in the proportion who delayed presentation up until the 1990s (Westcombe et al, 1999); we found that only 8% of breast cancer patients delayed presentation. There is also evidence for gastrointestinal cancers and urological cancer that patients, over time, are presenting more promptly after onset of symptoms (MacDonald et al, 2004). This may be due to increased awareness of cancer symptoms and confidence to seek help, and broader social changes in the nature of public discourse about health and cancer, including more openness and less stigma.

We did not find strong evidence that worry about wasting the doctor’s time or embarrassment can explain the high proportion of people who report delay in presentation. This contrasts with the findings of a 2011 survey of people aged 50+ in the UK, in which 34% reported that worry about wasting the doctor’s time and 15% that embarrassment might put them off going to a doctor with a symptom that might be serious (Forbes et al, 2013). What healthy people think would deter them going to the doctor may not be the same as what deters them when they actually experience a symptom.

This study highlights that interventions should ensure that they are designed to work for people from the lowest socioeconomic groups – although our findings suggest that delay in presentation is not uncommon in people from higher socioeconomic groups. Approaches to promoting early presentation should aim to raise awareness of the significance of certain symptoms, in particular rectal bleeding, because there is a high risk of delay in presentation, a high positive predictive value for cancer (Hamilton, 2009) and a low risk of overdiagnosis of indolent tumours (Welch and Black, 2010). Promoting awareness of some of the less-specific symptoms, however, such as urinary difficulties in older men, may expose people to the risk of overdiagnosis of indolent tumours that would never have caused harm, or unnecessary tests and anxiety. Our study can contribute to policy on promoting early diagnosis of cancer, and to the government’s ambition to improve cancer survival in the UK.