Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • ONLINE FIRST
  • CURRENT ISSUE
  • ALL ISSUES
  • AUTHORS & REVIEWERS
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • BJGP LIFE
  • MORE
    • About BJGP
    • Conference
    • Advertising
    • eLetters
    • Alerts
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Librarian information
    • Resilience
    • COVID-19 Clinical Solutions
  • RCGP
    • BJGP for RCGP members
    • BJGP Open
    • RCGP eLearning
    • InnovAIT Journal
    • Jobs and careers

User menu

  • Subscriptions
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
British Journal of General Practice
Intended for Healthcare Professionals
  • RCGP
    • BJGP for RCGP members
    • BJGP Open
    • RCGP eLearning
    • InnovAIT Journal
    • Jobs and careers
  • Subscriptions
  • Alerts
  • Log in
  • Follow bjgp on Twitter
  • Visit bjgp on Facebook
  • Blog
  • Listen to BJGP podcast
  • Subscribe BJGP on YouTube
Intended for Healthcare Professionals
British Journal of General Practice

Advanced Search

  • HOME
  • ONLINE FIRST
  • CURRENT ISSUE
  • ALL ISSUES
  • AUTHORS & REVIEWERS
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • BJGP LIFE
  • MORE
    • About BJGP
    • Conference
    • Advertising
    • eLetters
    • Alerts
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Librarian information
    • Resilience
    • COVID-19 Clinical Solutions
Research

Verbal and non-verbal behaviour and patient perception of communication in primary care: an observational study

Paul Little, Peter White, Joanne Kelly, Hazel Everitt, Shkelzen Gashi, Annemieke Bikker and Stewart Mercer
British Journal of General Practice 2015; 65 (635): e357-e365. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgp15X685249
Paul Little
Primary Care Group, Primary Care and Population Sciences Unit, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton.
Roles: Professor of primary care research
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Peter White
Nightingale Surgery, Romsey.
Roles: GP
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joanne Kelly
Primary Care Group, Primary Care and Population Sciences Unit, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton.
Roles: Study manager
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hazel Everitt
Primary Care Group, Primary Care and Population Sciences Unit, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton.
Roles: Associate professor
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shkelzen Gashi
Primary Care Group, Primary Care and Population Sciences Unit, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton.
Roles: Medical student
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Annemieke Bikker
General Practice & Primary Care, Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University of Glasgow, Glasgow.
Roles: Research associate
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stewart Mercer
General Practice & Primary Care, Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University of Glasgow, Glasgow.
Roles: Professor of primary care research
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Background Few studies have assessed the importance of a broad range of verbal and non-verbal consultation behaviours.

Aim To explore the relationship of observer ratings of behaviours of videotaped consultations with patients’ perceptions.

Design and setting Observational study in general practices close to Southampton, Southern England.

Method Verbal and non-verbal behaviour was rated by independent observers blind to outcome. Patients competed the Medical Interview Satisfaction Scale (MISS; primary outcome) and questionnaires addressing other communication domains.

Results In total, 275/360 consultations from 25 GPs had useable videotapes. Higher MISS scores were associated with slight forward lean (an 0.02 increase for each degree of lean, 95% confidence interval [CI] = 0.002 to 0.03), the number of gestures (0.08, 95% CI = 0.01 to 0.15), ‘back-channelling’ (for example, saying ‘mmm’) (0.11, 95% CI = 0.02 to 0.2), and social talk (0.29, 95% CI = 0.4 to 0.54). Starting the consultation with professional coolness (‘aloof’) was helpful and optimism unhelpful. Finishing with non-verbal ‘cut-offs’ (for example, looking away), being professionally cool (‘aloof’), or patronising, (‘infantilising’) resulted in poorer ratings. Physical contact was also important, but not traditional verbal communication.

Conclusion These exploratory results require confirmation, but suggest that patients may be responding to several non-verbal behaviours and non-specific verbal behaviours, such as social talk and back-channelling, more than traditional verbal behaviours. A changing consultation dynamic may also help, from professional ‘coolness’ at the beginning of the consultation to becoming warmer and avoiding non-verbal cut-offs at the end.

  • communication
  • consultation
  • general practice
  • non-verbal communication

INTRODUCTION

The patient-centred consultation model is widely advocated,1,2 although implementation is possibly limited,3–5 and poor communication results in complaints and lawsuits.6 Communication skills training is also probably very efficient given the long-lasting effects of training.7–10

A Cochrane Review of trials to modify patient-centredness documented mixed effects on satisfaction and small effects on health status.11 However, for satisfaction, none of the studies addressed non-verbal skills, and nearly all were intensive (‘brief’ training was up to 10 hours).11 Most models of patient-centred behaviour refer to traditional verbal skills, but a review of 22 observational studies suggested that other important factors were courtesy, empathy, positive reinforcement, reassurance and support, psychosocial talk, friendliness, humour, explanations, summarising and clarification, a direct body orientation, symmetrical legs and arms, forward lead, nodding, and gaze.12 An observational study identified important domains of patients’ perceptions — a communication and partnership approach, interest in the patient’s life, health promotion, a positive approach, and a personal relationship — each of which strongly predicts different outcomes.13,14 However, what mixture of verbal and non-verbal elements of doctor behaviour determines patients’ perceptions is less clear.

The previous literature also has significant limitations, great variability in what is rated (few include even the limited variables assessed by Beck et al),12 and outcomes. In one review of trials, meta-analysis was not possible due to the heterogeneity of interventions and outcomes,15 and in the Cochrane Review updated in 2012 heterogeneity was also moderately high.11

This study aimed to explore which aspects of GPs’ non-verbal and verbal communication are likely to be most important in determining patients’ satisfaction and perceptions of person-centred communication in the consultation.

METHOD

Unselected GP consultations for consecutive patients were videotaped and verbal and non-verbal behaviour was rated by independent observers blind to outcome. Patients completed ratings post-consultation questionnaires using the Medical Interview Satisfaction Scale (MISS) and other scales measuring the patient-centredness of the consultation.

Participants

Participants were adult patients, or children attending with their parents, for a new or ongoing problem, that is, not those attending simply for a repeat prescription. There were no exclusions apart from the inability to consent or complete questionnaires; for example, those experiencing severe distress, dementia, very severe depression, or who were very severely unwell.

How this fits in

Communication is central to every consultation and although a range of key elements have been advocated, few previous studies have assessed the impact of a wide range of verbal and non-verbal behaviours. This study suggests that clinicians may need to pay at least as much attention to non-verbal behaviours and non-specific verbal behaviours, such as social talk and back-channelling, as traditional verbal behaviours. They should avoid optimism but maintain professional ‘coolness’ at the beginning of the consultation, and end the consultation with warmth, avoiding non-verbal cut-offs.

Recruitment

Consecutive patients were recruited by 25 GPs in general practices close to Southampton in Southern England. The GPs in turn were recruited by the local postgraduate coordinator close to the Southampton postgraduate centre. GPs were asked to recruit up to 15 consecutive patients each.

Rating of videotapes

Doctors were videotaped with the video camera pointed towards the doctor. Prior findings demonstrated that short consultation ‘slices’ correlate well to total ratings; this was confirmed for most items, but where poor correlations were observed (for example, for Roter items — Roter Interaction Analysis System — such as social talk), the whole consultation was used. Thus, unless specified, the first and last minutes of the consultation were rated. The middle of the consultation was not chosen to avoid examinations performed away from the camera. Ratings were blind to the questionnaire responses by a research assistant or member of the research team, using a battery of assessments. Particular assessments were chosen based on previous significant findings. Further details of rating of videotapes and descriptive data for key variables are available from the authors.

Patient questionnaires

A similar methodology was used to the previous study:14 a post-consultation questionnaire explored the patient’s perception of communication. Questionnaires were completed by participants with help from parents as appropriate, particularly for very young children, either immediately after the consultation or, more commonly, at home and then posted back.

Primary outcome

The Medical Interview Satisfaction Scale (MISS)16 was chosen since its domains predominantly reflect communication and the doctor–patient relationship (distress–relief; communication–comfort; rapport; and compliance–intent). Patients agree or disagree on a 7-point Likert scale (very strongly agree to very strongly disagree) with items about how they rated the consultation.

Secondary outcomes

Similar 7-point Likert scales were also completed for several domains of patients’ perceptions of communication previously validated:14

  • communication and partnership;

  • a ‘personal’ relationship;

  • health promotion;

  • a ‘positive’ approach — being definite about the problem and when it would settle; and

  • interest in the effect on life.

The questionnaire also contained sociodemographic details, the short state anxiety questionnaire,17 the Whitley Index, the number of medical problems, current medication, enablement,18 symptom burden (Measure Yourself Medical Outcome Profile),19 whether seeing their usual doctor, the complaint (based on British National Formulary chapters), and whether or not this was ongoing.

Sample size

The sample size was calculated using an α of 0.01 and β of 0.2 using the NQuery sample size programme version 3. It was estimated that to detect a correlation of 0.25 between patient perception of communication and verbal or non-verbal communication rated in the consultation required 183 patients, or 229 allowing for 20% incomplete outcomes. Therefore 0.25 was chosen to allow for some margin of error: a correlation of 0.29 was observed between verbal behaviour and satisfaction in a similar previous UK study.20

Analysis

The data were analysed using SPSS (for data manipulation) and Stata (for modelling) statistical software for Windows™ using multiple linear regression, controlling for clustering by GP (Stata version 12; SPSS version 21). The linearity of associations was checked graphically and using ordered categorical variables. The dependent variables were the patients’ rating of the consultations and the independent variables were ratings of the physician’s behaviour (both verbal and non-verbal), as well as the rest of the data from the questionnaires.

Variables were selected manually by forward selection (to limit the development of spurious findings from automated procedures), and variables retained if there was limited inflation of standard errors, and if they were found to be significant (P<0.05) in multivariate analysis. All variables were then also checked manually in the final model, to ensure no variables that could be important had been overlooked (and which could have been spuriously thrown out by an automated procedure). Missing values were not imputed.

RESULTS

Patients were recruited from February until April each year from 2006 until 2010 by 25 GPs: 9/25 (36%) female; 6/25 (24%) not partners; and 5/25 (20%) working in deprived inner city areas.

Most patients could not be approached because of insufficient time to consent prior to the consultation. Of those who were approached, most agreed to participate (60%), with the remainder either not having the time or inclination to participate (35%), or because of the sensitive nature of the consultation (5%).

Of the 360 patients who initially agreed, 275 useable videotapes could be rated, and of these, 251/275 (91%) have useable questionnaire data, that is, a MISS questionnaire could be calculated.

The mean age of the index patients was 48 years, 138/215 (64%) were female, 145/223 (65%) were married, 13/197 (7%) were in receipt of sickness or disability benefit, 111/228 (49%) were in paid work, and had on average been to the doctor five times in the previous 12 months.

Patients rated both satisfaction and communication in the consultation highly: the mean item score for MISS on a 1–7 scale was 5.6 (standard deviation [SD] = 0.8), for the communication and partnership scale 5.5 (SD = 0.8), the personal relationship scale 5.1 (SD = 1.3), and the interest in life scale 5.2 (SD = 1.2).

Table 1 shows the results for the MISS questionnaire. Patients’ rating of satisfaction (mean item score on the MISS questionnaire, scaled 1–7) increased with slight lean towards the patient, the number of gestures, and ‘back-channel prompts’ (such as saying ‘mmm’, ‘ah ha’, and so on), at the beginning. Social talk at some point in the consultation was associated with increased satisfaction, and infantilising (being patronising) at the end of the consultation was associated with a negative impact.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Variables (scored from videotapes) significantly associated with the mean item score of the MISS questionnaire (rated by patients after the consultation; each item scored 1–7)

Tables 2⇓–4 document the associations for the key communication domains. Information on the perception of health promotion, a positive approach, and enablement domains are shown in Appendices 1–3. Variables with estimates that were either significant in both univariate analysis and multivariate analysis, or significant in multivariate analysis but a consistent direction in both univariate and multivariate analysis, are highlighted in Table 5. The pattern of findings in Table 5 suggest being aloof, dominant, or infantilising at the end of the consultation, or using non-verbal cutoffs were associated with several domains of negative perceptions of communication by patients. The negative effect of being aloof or infantilising at the end of the consultation must be put in the context that for only a minority of consultations was there a negative rating: 23/268 (9%), and 32/268 (12%), respectively. Being physically engaged (using gestures or appropriate touch) and socially engaged (social talk) had positive effects. At the beginning of the consultation being supportive was helpful, but optimism was not. Conversely, being professionally aloof at the beginning of the consultation was helpful; suggesting a cool but supportive listening approach, without injecting artificial optimism at too early a stage, could be optimal.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Variables associated with patient rating of a communication and partnership approach

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Variables associated with patient rating of a personal relationship

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Variables associated with patient rating of perceived interest of the doctor in their life

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 5.

Summary of significant positive (beneficial outcome) and negative (adverse outcome) associations of key variables and whether the variable rating refers to the beginning, end, or whole consultation

DISCUSSION

Summary

This exploratory study is one of the largest to assess a range of verbal and non-verbal behaviours. It suggests that several non-verbal behaviours and non-specific verbal behaviours, such as social talk and back-channelling, may impact more than traditional verbal behaviours. It also proposes that a changing consultation dynamic may be important — from being professionally supportive but ‘cooler’ at the beginning to being warmer and avoiding non-verbal cut-offs at the end.

Strengths and limitations

This was one of the largest studies to assess a very broad range of verbal and non-verbal behaviours in primary care.

The main potential limitations of this study are confounding and type I errors. Type I error is less likely for variables that had multiple associations, and more likely for variables only associated with one outcome and only in multivariate analysis (for example, the rate of speech) or with mixed effects (for example, lean towards the patient and being definite about the nature of the problem). Confounding was controlled as necessary for age, sex of the doctor, type of problem, being in receipt of sickness and unemployment benefit, whether the patient was seeing their usual doctor, and attitudes to doctors.

Even with the range of variables controlled for it is difficult to deal with residual confounding, and there is also the danger of over-fitting. However variables, such as non-verbal cut-offs or being aloof or infantilising, which were significant and had similar estimates in both univariate and multivariate analysis (that is, stable estimates and less likely to be confounded), provide stronger evidence of causality.

Conversely, concerns about confounding are stronger when the estimates are unstable, hence variables with more consistent estimates were highlighted. Type II error (β) is also possible, even though this is one of the largest studies to assess such a complete range of variables (very large studies are challenging due to the very intensive nature of the video assessment). Reverse causality is likely to be relevant for some variables, for example, psychosocial talk (which is likely to occur in consultations with more distress and emotion expressed) was associated with negative perception of communication. Although Rosenthal et al have shown that naïve raters can use their scales reliably,21–23 judgements about each item will inevitably be subjective, although the impact of a single rater’s judgement was minimised by using several raters. Similarly, despite using the Stewart method which has an extensive manual,2 the estimation of reliability in this study suggests that for many variables reliability is only likely to be moderate, and so the associations may have been underestimated. Booking interval was not controlled for but total time in the consultations was not an important predictor.

These results should be confirmed in a wider group of GPs: although patients from GPs working in deprived areas had slightly higher MISS ratings and a ‘positive’ doctor approach, and partners had higher ratings than non-partners, these variables were controlled for in analysis. Selection bias potentially applies to both doctor and patient: Mead and Bower suggested that doctors who consider themselves to be good communicators, are thus likely to have better verbal communication skills and are more likely to take part, which will have potentially underestimated the importance of verbal skills.24 Patients with a sensitive nature to their problem/s are more reluctant to be filmed, and it is in such areas where patient-centredness is likely to be most important.24

Recruitment and rating of videotapes was slow due to the day-to-day running being coordinated each year by medical students as part of their research projects, and students could only provide concentrated input for 1–2 months per year. The requirement to use students in part reflects the difficulty of obtaining funding for this research. Furthermore, due to the logistic requirement to approach patients and obtain consent, a large proportion of consultations were not recorded.

Comparison with existing literature

Several variables in the current study were apparently important but only at the end of the consultation. The importance of avoiding non-verbal cut-offs supports Mehrabian,25,26 and suggests that non-verbal cut-offs give the patient the impression that the doctors communication skills are poor and that the doctor is not interested in their life. As might be expected, avoiding any sense of distance at the end of the consultation, such as aloof or infantilising/patronising,22,23 was powerful, although few doctor consultations in this study were judged to be very aloof or infantilising. The apparently surprising findings of some benefit from being ‘aloof’ and not overly optimistic at the beginning of the consultation suggests a changing dynamic throughout the consultation. This may reflect the importance early in the consultation of a cooler but supportive professional manner in helping patients feel listened to. The use of gestures and touch, that is, appropriate physical contact, are both supported by previous work,26,27 and were consistent between univariate and multivariate analysis.

The use of social conversations was important and is one of the central measures in the Roter interaction analysis system28,29 Where there was social conversation, patients were more likely to feel there was a communication and partnership approach, a personal relationship, and that the doctor was positive. Some of this may be reverse causality given that patients who know their doctor better are more likely to be engaged in social conversation and vice versa. However, controlling for whether this was the patient’s normal doctor did not modify the estimates, so reverse causality seems a less likely explanation. As expected demonstrating knowledge of the patient and their history was relevant for patients having a sense of a personal relationship, and this finding was also not affected by whether the doctor was the patient’s usual doctor. This highlights the importance when the GP is not the usual doctor of quickly checking the key elements of the patient’s past history.

Conventional approaches in conceptualising verbal aspects of patient-centredness — exploring the disease, understanding the person as a whole, and finding common ground — were not strong findings. This is closer to Mead and Bower who found very limited associations,24 but the correlations in Kinnersley et al’s study were also not large.20 This may reflect the limited range of GPs — studies of communication tend to enlist those already most interested in communication — but the range of scores for verbal communication perhaps makes this explanation less likely.

Implications for research and practice

Given the exploratory nature of this study, and the likely self-selection of GPs, very firm recommendations cannot be made. However, if these results can be confirmed, they suggest that health professionals should pay attention to non-verbal skills in the consultation, particularly the use of gestures and physical contact, and to non-specific verbal elements such as back-channel prompts and social conversation. A professionally cool approach may be helpful at the beginning of the consultation, but not at the end where a warmer approach and particular care to avoid non-verbal cut-offs are needed.

Acknowledgments

On behalf of the patient communication project team: Marinar Paramour, Anna Jackson, Olayiwola Olatawura, Becky Hawes, Emma Marston, Jennifer Walsh, Hannah Ebdon, Corrina Quinney and Fiona Hignett (all medical students at the time).

Appendix 1. Variables associated with patient rating of health promotion

Predictor variablesUnivariateP-valueMultivariateP-value
Non verbal
Non verbal cut–off occurred (end consultation)−2.28 (−3.20 to −1.37)<0.001−2.89 (−5.35 to −0.43)0.023
Object manipulation (end consultation)−0.13 (−0.27 to 0.01)0.074−0.21 (−0.38 to −0.04)0.020
Computer use (beginning and end combined)−0.34 (−0.70 to 0.02)0.066−0.59 (−1.02 to −0.16)0.010
Verbal
Feedback examination (findings unsolicited)0.001 (−0.08 to 0.08)0.9760.41 (0.13 to 0.68)0.006
Overall impression
Aloof (beginning consultation)0.07 (−0.17 to 0.31)0.5460.34 (0.21 to 0.48)<0.001
Aloof (end consultation)−0.04 (−0.36 to 0.29)0.816−0.28 (−0.49 to −0.07)0.011
Infantilising (end consultation)−0.34 (−0.88 to 0.20)0.210−0.69 (−1.15 to −0.23)0.005
  • Univariate (n = 224) controlling for clustering by doctor. Multivariate (n = 195) controlling for clustering by doctor, type of problem, history time, total number of medical problems, and for all significant predictors listed.

Appendix 2. Variables associated with patient rating of the doctor being positive about the problem and its natural history

Predictor variablesUnivariateP-valueMultivariateP-value
Non verbal
Negative facial expression (end consultation)−2.13 (−3.53 to −0.72)0.005−2.06 (−3.61 to −0.52)0.011
Lean towards the patient (end consultation)−0.01 (−0.02 to 0.01)0.002−0.02 (−0.03 to −0.01)0.006
Physical contact occurred during whole consultation2.39 (0.53 to 4.23)0.0142.52 (0.03 to 5.00)0.047
Negative head movement (beginning consultation)1.05 (0.58 to 1.52)<0.0011.22 (0.62 to 1.82)<0.001
Verbal
How definite about natural history0.12 (−0.01 to 0.25)0.0770.13 (0.02 to 0.23)0.021
Social talk during whole consultation0.28 (−0.14 to 0.70)0.1770.62 (0.10 to 1.13)0.021
Psychosocial talk during whole consultation−0.20 (−0.48 to 0.07)0.135−0.55 (−0.88 to −0.22)0.002
Open questions (end consultation)−1.23 (−2.69 to 0.23)0.095−1.35 (−2.65 to −0.04)0.044
Overall impression
Aloof (beginning consultation)0.01 (−0.28 to 0.29)0.9680.17 (0.05 to 0.29)0.007
Aloof (end consultation)−0.18 (−0.64 to 0.29)0.437−0.34 (−0.64 to −0.04)0.026
  • Univariate controlling for clustering by doctor. Multivariate controlling for clustering by doctor, type of problem, deprived practice, state anxiety and for all significant predictors listed.

Appendix 3. Variables associated with patient rating of enablement

Predictor variablesUnivariateP-valueMultivariateP-value
Non verbal
Lean (beginning consultation)−0.01 (−0.02 to 0.00)0.041−0.01 (−0.02 to 0.00)0.045
Match of rate of speech (end consultation)−1.08 (−1.94 to − 0.21)0.017−1.92 (−3.22 to −0.62)0.006
Match of patient and doctor tone (beginning consultation)−0.19 (−0.53 to 0.15)0.262−0.35 (−0.52 to 0.17)0.001
Verbal
Feedback examination−0.01 (−0.05 to 0.02)0.463−0.04 (−0.07 to −0.01)0.013
Definite about the problem−0.03 (−0.06 to −0.01)0.167−0.05 (−0.09 to −0.02)0.005
Overall impression
Hostile (beginning consultation)−0.62 (−1.00 to −0.24)0.003−0.39 (−0.59 to −0.18)0.001
  • Univariate (n = 234) controlling for clustering by doctor. Multivariate (n = 206) controlling for clustering by doctor, type of problem, deprived practice, attitude to doctors and for all significant predictors listed.

Notes

Funding

We are grateful to the Scientific Foundation of the RCGP and the NIHR South West Regional R+D panel for partly funding this work. (Reference number SFB 2003/44).

Ethical approval

The study was approved by the Salisbury and South East Hampshire local research ethics committees (Southampton Local Research Ethics Committee number: 230/97).

Provenance

Freely submitted; externally peer reviewed.

Competing interests

The authors have declared no competing interests.

Discuss this article

Contribute and read comments about this article: bjgp.org/letters

  • Received August 23, 2014.
  • Revision requested October 6, 2014.
  • Accepted November 13, 2014.
  • © British Journal of General Practice 2015

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    1. Stewart MA
    (1995) Effective physician-patient communication and health outcomes: a review. CMAJ 152(9):1423–1433.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  2. 2.↵
    1. Brown JB,
    2. Stewart M,
    3. Tessier S
    (1995) Assessing communication between patients and doctors: a manual for scoring patient-centred communication. Working Paper Series 95-2 (Centre for Studies in Family Medicine and Thames Valley Family Practice Research Unit, London).
  3. 3.↵
    1. Barry CA,
    2. Bradley CP,
    3. Britten N,
    4. et al.
    (2000) Patients’ unvoiced agendas in general practice consultations: qualitative study. BMJ 320(7244):1246–1250.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  4. 4.
    1. Britten N,
    2. Stevenson FA,
    3. Barry CA,
    4. et al.
    (2000) Misunderstandings in prescribing decisions in general practice: qualitative study. BMJ 320(7233):484–488.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  5. 5.↵
    1. Law SA,
    2. Britten N
    (1995) Factors that influence the patient centredness of a consultation. Br J Gen Pract 45(399):520–524.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. 6.↵
    1. Meryn S
    (1998) Improving doctor–patient communication. Not an option, but a necessity. BMJ 316(7149):1922–1930.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  7. 7.↵
    1. Maguire P,
    2. Fairbairn S,
    3. Fletcher C
    (1986) Consultation skills of young doctors: Benefits of feedback training in interviewing as students persist. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 292(6535):1573–1576.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. 8.
    1. Little P,
    2. Stuart B,
    3. Moore M,
    4. et al.
    (2013) Amoxicillin for acute lower-respiratory-tract infection in primary care when pneumonia is not suspected: a 12-country, randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Infect Dis 13(2):123–129.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.
    1. Butler CC,
    2. Simpson SA,
    3. Dunstan F,
    4. et al.
    (2012) Effectiveness of multifaceted educational programme to reduce antibiotic dispensing in primary care: practice based randomised controlled trial. BMJ doi:10.1136/bmj.d8173.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. 10.↵
    1. Cals JW,
    2. de Bock L,
    3. Beckers PJ,
    4. et al.
    (2013) Enhanced communication skills and C-reactive protein point-of-care testing for respiratory tract infection: 3.5-year follow-up of a cluster randomized trial. Ann Fam Med 11(2):157–164.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  11. 11.↵
    1. Dwamena F,
    2. Holmes-Rovner M,
    3. Gaulden CM,
    4. et al.
    (2012) Interventions for providers to promote a patient-centred approach in clinical consultations. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 12(12):CD003267.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Beck RS,
    2. Daughtridge R,
    3. Sloane PD
    (2002) Physician-patient communication in the primary care office: a systematic review. J Am Board Fam Pract 15(1):25–38.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  13. 13.↵
    1. Little P,
    2. Everitt H,
    3. Williamson I,
    4. et al.
    (2001) Preferences of patients for patient centred approach to consultation in primary care: observational study. BMJ 322(7284):468–472.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. 14.↵
    1. Little P,
    2. Everitt H,
    3. Williamson I,
    4. et al.
    (2001) Obervational study of effect of patient centredness and positive approach on outcomes of general practice consultations. BMJ 323(7318):908–911.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. 15.↵
    1. Griffin SJ,
    2. Kinmonth AL,
    3. Veltman MW,
    4. et al.
    (2004) Effect on health-related outcomes of interventions to alter the interaction between patients and practitioners: a systematic review of trials. Ann Fam Med 2(6):595–608.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  16. 16.↵
    1. Hallam D,
    2. Doggett L,
    3. Wilkin MA
    (1992) Measures of need and outcome for primary health care (Oxford University Press, Oxford).
  17. 17.↵
    1. Marteau TM,
    2. Bekker H
    (1992) The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Br J Clin Psychol 31(3):301–306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Howie JG,
    2. Heaney DJ,
    3. Maxwell M,
    4. et al.
    (1999) Quality at general practice consultations: cross sectional survey. BMJ 319(7212):738–743.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. 19.↵
    1. Paterson C
    (1996) Measuring outcomes in primary care: a patient generated measure, MYMOP, compared with the SF-36 health survey. BMJ 312(7037):1016–1020.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. 20.↵
    1. Kinnersley P,
    2. Stott N,
    3. Peters TJ,
    4. Harvey I
    (1999) The patient-centredness of consultations and outcome in primary care. Br J Gen Pract 49(446):711–716.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. 21.↵
    1. Ambady N,
    2. Laplante D,
    3. Nguyen T,
    4. et al.
    (2002) Surgeons’ tone of voice: a clue to malpractice history. Surgery 132(1):5–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. 22.↵
    1. Ambady N,
    2. Koo J,
    3. Rosenthal R,
    4. Winograd CH
    (2002) Physical therapists’ nonverbal communication predicts geriatric patients’ health outcomes. Psychol Aging 17(3):443–452.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    1. Ambady N,
    2. Rosenthal R
    (1992) Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 111(2):256–274.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. 24.↵
    1. Mead N,
    2. Bower P
    (2000) Measuring patient-centredness: a comparison of three observation-based instruments. Patient Educ Couns 39(1):71–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Mehrabian A
    (1972) Nonverbal communication (Aldine Atherton Inc. Chicago, IL).
  26. 26.↵
    1. Mehrabian A
    (1968) Some referents and measures of nonverbal behavior. Behavior Res Meth Instrument 1:203–207.
    OpenUrl
  27. 27.↵
    1. Gleeson M,
    2. Timmins F
    (2005) A review of the use and clinical effectiveness of touch as a nursing intervention. Clin Eff Nurs 9:69–77.
    OpenUrl
  28. 28.↵
    1. Roter D,
    2. Larson S
    (2002) The Roter interaction analysis system (RIAS): utility and flexibility for analysis of medical interactions. Patient Educ Couns 46(4):243–251.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Sandvik M,
    2. Eide H,
    3. Lind M,
    4. et al.
    (2002) Analyzing medical dialogues: strength and weakness of Roter’s interaction analysis system (RIAS). Patient Educ Couns 46(4):235–241.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

In this issue

British Journal of General Practice: 65 (635)
British Journal of General Practice
Vol. 65, Issue 635
June 2015
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Or,
sign in or create an account with your email address
Email Article

Thank you for recommending British Journal of General Practice.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person to whom you are recommending the page knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Verbal and non-verbal behaviour and patient perception of communication in primary care: an observational study
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from British Journal of General Practice
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from British Journal of General Practice.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Verbal and non-verbal behaviour and patient perception of communication in primary care: an observational study
Paul Little, Peter White, Joanne Kelly, Hazel Everitt, Shkelzen Gashi, Annemieke Bikker, Stewart Mercer
British Journal of General Practice 2015; 65 (635): e357-e365. DOI: 10.3399/bjgp15X685249

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Share
Verbal and non-verbal behaviour and patient perception of communication in primary care: an observational study
Paul Little, Peter White, Joanne Kelly, Hazel Everitt, Shkelzen Gashi, Annemieke Bikker, Stewart Mercer
British Journal of General Practice 2015; 65 (635): e357-e365. DOI: 10.3399/bjgp15X685249
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Jump to section

  • Top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • INTRODUCTION
    • METHOD
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgments
    • Appendix 1. Variables associated with patient rating of health promotion
    • Appendix 2. Variables associated with patient rating of the doctor being positive about the problem and its natural history
    • Appendix 3. Variables associated with patient rating of enablement
    • Notes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • communication
  • consultation
  • general practice
  • non-verbal communication

More in this TOC Section

  • Introducing genetic testing with case finding for familial hypercholesterolaemia in primary care: qualitative study of patient and health professional experience
  • Impact of COVID-19 on primary care contacts with children and young people in England: longitudinal trends study 2015–2020
  • Non-speculum clinician-taken samples for human papillomavirus testing: a cross-sectional study in older women
Show more Research

Related Articles

Cited By...

Intended for Healthcare Professionals

BJGP Life

BJGP Open

 

@BJGPjournal's Likes on Twitter

 
 

British Journal of General Practice

NAVIGATE

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • All Issues
  • Online First
  • Authors & reviewers

RCGP

  • BJGP for RCGP members
  • BJGP Open
  • RCGP eLearning
  • InnovAiT Journal
  • Jobs and careers

MY ACCOUNT

  • RCGP members' login
  • Subscriber login
  • Activate subscription
  • Terms and conditions

NEWS AND UPDATES

  • About BJGP
  • Alerts
  • RSS feeds
  • Facebook
  • Twitter

AUTHORS & REVIEWERS

  • Submit an article
  • Writing for BJGP: research
  • Writing for BJGP: other sections
  • BJGP editorial process & policies
  • BJGP ethical guidelines
  • Peer review for BJGP

CUSTOMER SERVICES

  • Advertising
  • Contact subscription agent
  • Copyright
  • Librarian information

CONTRIBUTE

  • BJGP Life
  • eLetters
  • Feedback

CONTACT US

BJGP Journal Office
RCGP
30 Euston Square
London NW1 2FB
Tel: +44 (0)20 3188 7400
Email: journal@rcgp.org.uk

British Journal of General Practice is an editorially-independent publication of the Royal College of General Practitioners
© 2022 British Journal of General Practice

Print ISSN: 0960-1643
Online ISSN: 1478-5242