Skip to main content

Main menu

  • HOME
  • ONLINE FIRST
  • CURRENT ISSUE
  • ALL ISSUES
  • AUTHORS & REVIEWERS
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • BJGP LIFE
  • MORE
    • About BJGP
    • Conference
    • Advertising
    • eLetters
    • Alerts
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Librarian information
    • Resilience
    • COVID-19 Clinical Solutions
  • RCGP
    • BJGP for RCGP members
    • BJGP Open
    • RCGP eLearning
    • InnovAIT Journal
    • Jobs and careers

User menu

  • Subscriptions
  • Alerts
  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
British Journal of General Practice
Intended for Healthcare Professionals
  • RCGP
    • BJGP for RCGP members
    • BJGP Open
    • RCGP eLearning
    • InnovAIT Journal
    • Jobs and careers
  • Subscriptions
  • Alerts
  • Log in
  • Follow bjgp on Twitter
  • Visit bjgp on Facebook
  • Blog
  • Listen to BJGP podcast
  • Subscribe BJGP on YouTube
British Journal of General Practice
Intended for Healthcare Professionals

Advanced Search

  • HOME
  • ONLINE FIRST
  • CURRENT ISSUE
  • ALL ISSUES
  • AUTHORS & REVIEWERS
  • SUBSCRIBE
  • BJGP LIFE
  • MORE
    • About BJGP
    • Conference
    • Advertising
    • eLetters
    • Alerts
    • Video
    • Audio
    • Librarian information
    • Resilience
    • COVID-19 Clinical Solutions
Research

Patients’ descriptions of the relation between physical symptoms and negative emotions: a qualitative analysis of primary care consultations

Ella Bekhuis, Janna Gol, Christopher Burton and Judith Rosmalen
British Journal of General Practice 2020; 70 (691): e78-e85. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgp19X707369
Ella Bekhuis
University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation (ICPE), University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands; Academic Unit of Primary Medical Care, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.
Roles: General medical doctor
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Janna Gol
University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, ICPE, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
Roles: Psychiatrist
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Christopher Burton
Academic Unit of Primary Medical Care, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.
Roles: GP and professor of primary medical care
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Judith Rosmalen
University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, ICPE, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
Roles: Professor of psychosomatic medicine
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Background Primary care guidelines for the management of persistent, often ‘medically unexplained’, physical symptoms encourage GPs to discuss with patients how these symptoms relate to negative emotions. However, many GPs experience difficulties in reaching a shared understanding with patients.

Aim To explore how patients with persistent symptoms describe their negative emotions in relation to their physical symptoms in primary care consultations, in order to help GPs recognise the patient’s starting points in such discussions.

Design and setting A qualitative analysis of 47 audiorecorded extended primary care consultations with 15 patients with persistent physical symptoms.

Method The types of relationships patients described between their physical symptoms and their negative emotions were categorised using content analysis. In a secondary analysis, the study explored whether patients made transitions between the types of relations they described through the course of the consultations.

Results All patients talked spontaneously about their negative emotions. Three main categories of relations between these emotions and physical symptoms were identified: separated (negation of a link between the two); connected (symptom and emotion are distinct entities that are connected); and inseparable (symptom and emotion are combined within a single entity). Some patients showed a transition between categories of relations during the intervention.

Conclusion Patients describe different types of relations between physical symptoms and negative emotions in consultations. Physical symptoms can be attributed to emotions when patients introduce this link themselves, but this link tends to be denied when introduced by the GP. Awareness of the ways patients discuss these relations could help GPs to better understand the patient’s view and, in this way, collaboratively move towards constructive explanations and symptom management strategies.

  • communication
  • consultation
  • emotions
  • medically unexplained symptoms
  • relation

INTRODUCTION

Persistent physical symptoms are prevalent in the general population and are associated with reduced quality of life.1–3 Although these symptoms are often referred to as medically unexplained, they are increasingly recognised as representing complex interactions between peripheral and central processes.4,5 The management of the symptoms includes a combination of physical and psychological elements,6 and is often perceived as challenging by both patients and GPs.7,8 Patients highly value care that addresses the breadth of biopsychosocial aspects.9,10

One element of the management of persistent physical symptoms focuses on negative emotions.2,11 These emotions have the potential to play a part in worsening or maintaining physical symptoms, in addition to being a response to the symptoms.4,12 Furthermore, patients with physical symptoms as well as negative emotions report more functional and social limitations than patients without these emotions.1 Therefore, primary care guidelines recommend that GPs address the relation of these negative emotions with symptoms.11,13,14 Nevertheless, many GPs experience difficulties in arriving at a shared understanding with patients about this relation.15–17 In particular, when GPs introduce inappropriate or premature psychosocial links, these are typically rejected by patients.15,16,18,19 It has been suggested that this tension is related to the embedding of ‘medically unexplained symptoms’ in psychiatric rather than somatic classification systems, and this dualism leads patients to feel that the legitimacy of their symptoms is under threat.17

Several authors have proposed that a shared understanding about the relation between symptoms and emotions should be formed while using the patients’ starting point as a basis.13,20–22 In a process of constantly seeking agreement and adjusting explanations, the GP and patient can collaboratively broaden the conversation to other types of relations and, as such, formulate rich explanatory models.13,20,22 However, despite the existence of theoretical models that refer to thought patterns of patients regarding the relation between physical symptoms and emotions,20 the authors were unable to find a classification of how patients describe it in consultations.

This study aimed to classify the types of relations between physical symptoms and emotions that patients describe in primary care consultations. A secondary aim was to examine if patients moved between types of relations over time, to examine if the classification can be used to monitor a change in their presentation during interventions. A qualitative analysis was conducted of a series of extended consultations with specially trained GPs for patients with multiple persistent physical symptoms.14

Primary care guidelines for managing persistent ‘medically unexplained’ physical symptoms recommend discussing the links between physical symptoms and negative emotions. When GPs propose these links, patients often find them unhelpful or actively block them. This study examined patients’ own descriptions of the links between physical symptoms and negative emotions, and found that patients articulated three types of associations: separated (negation that symptom and emotion were related); connected (symptom and emotion were presented as distinct but connected entities); and inseparable (symptom and emotion were combined within one entity). Understanding how patients describe associations between physical symptoms and negative emotions may help GPs to find common ground for communication and enable appropriate symptom management strategies to be constructed.

How this fits in

METHOD

Data source

For the current study, data from the Multiple Symptoms Study 1 and 214,23 were extracted. These studies focused on the effects of a consultation intervention in primary care for patients with multiple persistent physical symptoms. This intervention, consisting of three to four consultations of 20–40 minutes with trained GPs, aimed to reduce the intensity and impact of symptoms.14,23 GPs were instructed to explore emotions when openings were presented by the patient, rather than to impose their own ideas upon the patient. Furthermore, they were encouraged to consider emotions as parallel processes that can be connected to physical symptoms, rather than presenting them as the sole cause or label of symptoms. In both studies, patients were identified through a clinical database search in their usual GP practice and the completion of the Patient Health Questionnaire-15 (PHQ-15; or its shortened 14-item version) to assess the severity of physical symptoms.24 Patients were eligible for inclusion if they had a diagnostic code in the clinical database for one or more functional somatic syndromes, had been referred to specialists at least two times in the preceding 3 years, and had a PHQ-15 score of ≥10. At study entry, patients filled in the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) and the Generalised Anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) to assess depressive and anxiety symptoms, respectively.24,25

Data analysis

Selection of consultations

All 112 consultations with 39 patients in which the intervention was delivered were audio-taped and transcribed verbatim. Patients were purposively sampled based on key variables (that is, age, sex, baseline scores on the PHQ-15, PHQ-9, and GAD-7, and the treating GP) to maximise variation. As one aspect of the study was the transition between different types of relations through the course of their consultations, patients who completed at least three consultations were particularly focused on. The analyses began on a subset of 12 patients and aimed for saturation defined as being no new insights on types and characteristics of categories gained in three sequentially analysed patients. As the inclusion of three additional cases did not provide additional insights, the final sample constituted 15 patients (Table 1).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Sample characteristics

Analysis method

First, patients’ accounts of negative emotions were coded in line with methods used in previous studies.26,27 These accounts were defined as explicit and verbal expressions of a negative emotional state. Implicit accounts of emotions (for example, a situational description of a distressing event such as a conflict without explicitly describing an emotion) were excluded to avoid imputation of the patient’s narrative by the researchers. Descriptions were interpreted in the context of the conversation and while listening to the intonation of the patient. Specific attention was paid to words or phrases with multiple definitions, for example, ‘stress’ can refer to external stressors as well as an internal state characterised by worry or agitation, and ‘I was like: oh my God!’ may refer to a positive and negative emotion. Such quotations were included only when it could be inferred with confidence that the patient was referring to a negative emotion. Accounts were categorised based on the type of emotion they concerned with open coding, and names of the categories were formulated while staying true to the words most often used by patients.

In a second step, all quotes in which patients described a relation between a negative emotion and physical symptom were selected. To stay true to the patient’s intent, only relations that were semantically specified were considered (for example, ‘I feel down because of the pain’). In addition, it had to be clear that the relations included a negative emotion as well as a physical symptom; terms at the interface (for example, ‘feeling tense’) were excluded if their meaning could not be inferred from the conversation. The quotes were analysed with conventional content analysis.28,29 Using a one-sheet-of-paper (OSOP) method, quotes were written on one document and rearranged by looking at similarities and differences to form categories inductively.30 To explore whether patients showed a transition in their presentation of categories through the course of their consultations, a secondary analysis was performed. Each patient’s pattern of category use over time was analysed, and switches from one category to another while describing a specific situation were searched for

Coding was done in ATLAS-ti 8 and first performed independently and then compared by two of the authors (EB and JG). The analysis was done by these researchers together and differences were discussed until agreement was reached. In order to ensure intersubjective reproducibility and comprehensibility, the analysis was regularly referred to senior researchers who specialised in persistent physical symptoms (CB and JR).

RESULTS

All patients described some negative emotions, but the number of instances differed considerably across patients (ranging from 3–21). Six categories of emotions were identified: anxiety, frustration, low mood, embarrassment, guilt, and other emotions that could not be fitted into these categories (Table 2). Patients who disclosed fewer emotions were generally less talkative and more focused on the physical aspects of their symptoms. Typically, patients presented the first emotion within 5 minutes of the start of the first consultation, and the frequency of occurrence decreased as the intervention progressed. In most quotes, patients related the emotion to physical symptoms (the amount ranged from 1 to 16 times). Patients initiated most descriptions of relations themselves, with the remainder in response to questioning and sometimes prompting by the GP. Spontaneous descriptions were more detailed than those occurring after a question from the GP. Some patients, particularly those going through major life events, tended to describe emotions without referring to their relation with their physical symptoms. They elaborated on emotions in the context of external stressors or questioned if the emotion was part of an affective disorder. The pattern in which patients presented emotions or their relation with physical symptoms (that is, number of times, types of categories, at which stage of the intervention) was not clearly related to the patients’ sex or age, nor the severity of somatic, depressive, and anxiety symptoms.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Categories of emotions

Categories of relations between negative emotions and physical symptoms

Three main categories of relations between negative emotions and physical symptoms were identified: separated, in which a link between the symptom and emotion was negated; connected, in which the symptom and emotion were presented as related but distinct entities; and inseparable, in which the symptom and emotion were combined within a single entity (Table 3).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Categories of relations between physical symptoms and emotions

Separated

In the separated category, patients explicitly negated a link between a symptom and an emotion. Characteristic for this category was that the negation concerned a relation in which the physical symptom was fully caused by or attributed to an emotion, and emotional labels like ‘depression’ or ‘anxiety disorder’ were used. Patients used the category during various stages of the intervention and frequently indicated that they believed the relation to be true in general, but that they had not experienced it themselves: ‘They looked at all the obvious signs because, I mean, they tested me physically, but they also looked at me emotionally as well, which is understandable. But I wasn’t going through any great emotional crisis and I wasn’t depressed and I wasn’t stressed and I have … I don’t know how most people work, but I’m a very strong faith so it keeps me sane, so I wasn’t … I would’ve told them if I was depressed and I wasn’t, so there wasn’t an emotional trigger.’(Patient [P] 12, female [F], age 20–34 years)

Some patients negated in a tense or angry way a relation suggested by a physician in which symptoms were caused by or attributed to emotions:

GP:‘They [pain and fatigue] are closely linked in with emotions and how all of that works, so being upset, being stressed, being angry.’ (GP4)

Patient (P):’Yeah, correlation between sad feelings and pain. I get it.’ (P12, F, age 20–34 years)

GP:‘So it starts to hold you back and you can get into a little bit of a cycle here.’

P:[starts crying and says angrily] ‘A rut, yeah, I appreciate that. But then if you couldn’t do half the things you wanted to do, you would feel overwhelmed and stressed out. But that’s not why I’m sore.’

Connected

The connected category included descriptions of a symptom and an emotion as distinct, yet related entities. This category included confidently presented statements that could lead to the identification of targets for management strategies. It was found during all stages of the intervention with all patients. Connections were subdivided into two types: a) isolated connection; and b) vicious circle.

In isolated connections, the symptom and the emotion either unidirectionally influenced each other or were linked in time. Typically, patients briefly described regularly experiencing the emotion as a consequence of the symptom, and in this way seemed to wish to underline the impact of the symptom on their daily life: ‘I still have this massive sweating, it’s a current one, it’s just very, very annoying, embarrassing, frustrating, depressing.’(P7, F, age 50–64 years) ‘Me stressing about her [patient’s sister] makes me not well. So I kind of have to go — well, not I don’t care — but if it is making me ill to stress about her, then I have to say: I’m just not going to.’(P4, F, age 50–64 years)

A vicious circle referred to a sequence of reciprocal cause and effect in which a symptom and an emotion intensified the effect of each other. Most patients described vicious circles after they had been introduced by the GP by briefly confirming the suggested relation (‘the pain … a little bit better’). However, a few patients, particularly those who described complex biopsychosocial explanations for their symptoms, spontaneously introduced vicious circles: ‘The pain, the heart things that you’re describing, and the shortness of breath, there’s no doubt to my mind that those are complicated processes at play. And everything that’s bad and making you feel depressed and making you feel down, that’s going to be filtering down, and making things worse. So that’s a vicious circle really, isn’t it?’(GP4) ‘That’s right, one that needs to be broken. How I don’t know, I really don’t know.’(P13, M, age 50–64 years) ‘I think that naturally leads us on to thinking about how we can make things a little bit better.’(GP4) ‘The headache adds to making me also tired because it wears you down. It’s not a — you know when you’ve got really bad headache that you go away and you get a paracetamol because it’s an ache — it’s not a throb. It’s just a continuous there dullness that wears you down, and when I get really tired, it starts to get quite bad. That’s more of a stabbing pain.’(P1, F, age 35–49 years)

Inseparable

Patients described a symptom and an emotion as combined within one entity in the inseparable category. This category was typically exploratory, included metaphors, and was introduced by patients at the beginning of the intervention. The symptom and emotion could be presented in two ways: an integrated whole, or fragments of a whole.

In descriptions of an integrated whole, the symptom and emotion were presented as one entity (that is, the symptom was part of the emotion or vice versa) (‘One night towards the end … pain of the brain’). This category concerned an exploration of the source of the symptom, which was typically an affective disorder: ‘I’ve been on a heart monitor and everything, but they haven’t come up with anything, so whether it’s a psychological thing or just some kind of panic attack?’(P11, F, age 35–49 years)

Most patients referred to their previous experiences with affective disorders: ‘At one stage I just thought: is it depression again? Because I’ve been through it before.’(P3, F, 35–49 years). ‘One night towards the end I woke up at 2.15 with this problem that’s been harassing me for the last 2 years with my sister. And because of the meditation it was bringing it up. I had such a pain in my head with it, the worry was very painful. And so I sat on the end of the bed and started to do the “scanning of the body”-meditation, and eventually overcame the pain of the brain.’(P11, F, age 35–49 years)

In the ‘fragments of a whole’ category, patients described the symptom and the emotion as inseparable features of an experience. The quotations included a chaotic narrative of a distressing state and patients were searching for the right words to describe it. The quotations were part of an active process of trying to understand the nature of the experience: ‘The bit I can’t work out is that I can just physically function all day and at some point it’s like I just … It’s like a wall hits me and it’s … And you can physically, I’ve been told you can physically … And I know that it’s hit me. I’ve been fine or I’ve been a bit tired all week, but Sunday night it was … I wasn’t doing anything and the wall hit me and I just … it’s like I just … I can’t cope with it. I can’t cope with anything and I have … it just … it’s like a … it’s like the … just the fatigue engulfs me.’(P1, F, age 35–49 years)

Transitions between categories

In a secondary analysis, the study explored whether patients could make a transition from one category to another through the course of their consultations. It was found that three patients presented one category, 11 described two or three categories, and one patient described five categories of relations.

However, most patients who described multiple categories referred to varying symptom–emotion combinations or contexts and therefore did not necessarily show an obvious change in their presentation during the intervention. Four instances were identified in which a patient showed an obvious transition in the presentation of a specific situation. This number was not sufficient to describe transitional patterns in detail.

In general, these transitions occurred in a dialogue in which the patient and GP negotiated novel types of relations. Two patterns of category switches were encountered: from a separated to an isolated connection; and from isolated connection to a vicious circle.

In the following quotes, the patient (P13, M, age 50–64 years) first describes an isolated connection, and later expands this, encouraged by the GP (GP4), to a vicious circle:

GP:‘And how are you feeling about all this [the pains], just as you are just now?’

P:‘Well depressed. What else can I say. I don’t know, just depressed, just feel like I’m getting nowhere.’ […]

GP:‘And can you see that they [these feelings] might be feeding back and, and making the symptoms worse as well?’ (GP4)

P:‘Possible, yes, very possible. That’s what I’m saying, my head’s maybe playing with my mind. My mind’s probably playing with me, making things worse. I work myself up, I get worse.’

DISCUSSION

Summary

This study showed that patients with persistent physical symptoms describe different types of relations between symptoms and emotions. Relationships constituted three main categories: separated (negation of a link), connected (the physical symptom and the emotion are two linked entities), and inseparable (the physical symptom and emotion are combined within one entity). Some patients moved from one category to another through the course of their consultations.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study are the dual independent coding and discussing of the analyses in a multidisciplinary team from general practice, psychiatry, and psychology.30 Furthermore, the study stayed true to the patients’ intent by focusing on explicit descriptions of emotions and their relation with physical symptoms. A limitation of this approach is that there may be some accounts that were missed where patients made implicit notice of emotions and/or relations; for example, by using terms at the interface of the physical and emotional (such as, ‘tense’).27 Additionally, although terms referring to physical or emotional aspects were inferred while staying as true as possible to the intent of the patient, it should be noted that this distinction is a simplification of the complex biopsychosocial reality.

Ambiguity with respect to the conceptual embedding of symptoms was extensively discussed before quotes were subjected to further analysis. Data were, however, derived from extended primary care consultations with specially trained GPs and not typical short GP consultations of 10 minutes. Although these long consultations were more likely than short consultations to include discussions involving the relation of physical symptoms with negative emotions, the passages of discussions were brief and so compatible with ‘ordinary’ consultations. Finally, as the study identified only four instances of clear transitions in patients’ use of categories over time, it was not possible to study their pattern in detail. Still, that such transitions occurred confirms that the categories can be used in future studies; for example, to identify interactional patterns related to transitions in patients’ presentations with conversation analysis.31

Comparison with existing literature

The finding that patients frequently present their emotions in primary care consultations is in line with previous studies.32–35 Although this study was the first to systematically assess the types of relations patients present in primary care consultations, other studies have indicated that many patients with persistent physical symptoms present their symptoms dualistically by negating a relation with emotions.18,19,36 Interestingly, in these studies the GPs primarily used classic psychological reattribution techniques,18,36 which centralise the assignment of emotional causes or labels to symptoms.37 This study found that patients forcibly rejected this in the separated category,18,36 suggesting that patients may primarily use dualistic expressions in response to reattribution by the GP. It has been reported before that many patients find reattribution too simplistic and stigmatising.9 This could partly explain the limited efficacy of interventions based on reattribution for persistent physical symptoms.11,38,39 Nevertheless, some patients in this study openly explored the possibility that their symptoms were part of an affective disorder in the inseparable category. This could indicate that patients can and do acknowledge emotional attribution or labels when they introduce them themselves, but tend to disagree when they are imposed upon them by the GP.20

Implications for practice

The results of this study have several implications for the care of patients with persistent physical symptoms. First, that patients spontaneously presented emotional states of anxiety, frustration, low mood, and embarrassment as well as guilt indicates the importance of considering a broad spectrum of emotions in consultations for persistent physical symptoms. However, clinical guidelines for the management of persistent physical symptoms encourage GPs mainly to concentrate on the narrow field of depressive and anxiety disorders,12,40,41 and a similar focus is adopted in screening instruments.24,25,42 As all patients in this study, irrespective of the severity of their depressive and anxiety symptoms, frequently presented emotions, the findings stress the importance of picking up on patients’ emotional cues and encouraging patients to elaborate on them.

Interestingly, the study found that patients tended to disclose fewer emotions as the intervention progressed. This might be related to the structure of the intervention, which gradually shifted the focus from symptom exploration to the creation of symptom management strategies. As the GP’s role became increasingly more dominant in the follow-up consultations to create such strategies, the space for the patient was naturally reduced. This indicates that, in ordinary consultations aimed at exploring the problem space, it is essential for GPs to create an open conversation in which they actively listen to and collaborate with the patient.43 Allowing patients the space to arrive at explanations themselves rather than imposing it on them could also help to create richer explanatory models,20,44 as relations that were mentioned spontaneously by patients were presented in more detail than those in response to directive questions and prompting by the GP.

Patients with persistent physical symptoms present a wide variety of negative emotions in extended primary care consultations. In contrast with previous research suggesting that patients have dualistic presentations, this study found that patients do not only separate emotions from physical symptoms, but also describe them as entities that are connected to, or inseparable from, these symptoms.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Inge Stortenbeker for sharing her expertise during the course of this research and Rachel Grob for sending relevant literature about the analysis method.

Notes

Funding

The Multiple Symptoms Study 1 and 2 were funded by the Chief Scientist Office (reference CZG/2/412 and CZH/4/495).

Ethical approval

The Multiple Symptoms Study 1 and 2 were approved by the Lothian Research Ethics Committee (reference 09/S1102/34) and the North East Scotland Ethics Committee (reference 14/NS/1014);14,23 these approvals also included a detailed analysis of the consultations as conducted in the current study. All participants gave informed consent.

Provenance

Freely submitted; externally peer reviewed.

Competing interests

The authors have declared no competing interests.

Discuss this article

Contribute and read comments about this article: bjgp.org/letters

  • Received February 10, 2019.
  • Revision requested April 3, 2019.
  • Accepted June 3, 2019.
  • © British Journal of General Practice 2020

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    1. de Waal MW,
    2. Arnold IA,
    3. Eekhof JA,
    4. van Hemert AM
    (2004) Somatoform disorders in general practice: prevalence, functional impairment and comorbidity with anxiety and depressive disorders. Br J Psychiatry 184:470–476.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. 2.↵
    1. Burton C
    (2003) Beyond somatisation: a review of the understanding and treatment of medically unexplained physical symptoms (MUPS). Br J Gen Pract 53(488):231–239.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. 3.↵
    1. McGorm K,
    2. Burton C,
    3. Weller D,
    4. et al.
    (2010) Patients repeatedly referred to secondary care with symptoms unexplained by organic disease: prevalence, characteristics and referral pattern. Fam Pract 27(5):479–486.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Henningsen P,
    2. Gündel H,
    3. Kop WJ,
    4. et al.
    (2018) Persistent physical symptoms as perceptual dysregulation: a neuropsychobehavioral model and its clinical implications. Psychosom Med 80(5):422–431.
    OpenUrl
  5. 5.↵
    1. Ongaro G,
    2. Kaptchuk TJ
    (2019) Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the Bayesian brain. Pain 160(1):1–4.
    OpenUrl
  6. 6.↵
    1. Olde Hartman TC,
    2. Rosendal M,
    3. Aamland A,
    4. et al.
    (2017) BJGP Open, What do guidelines and systematic reviews tell us about the management of medically unexplained symptoms in primary care? DOI: https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgpopen17X101061.
  7. 7.↵
    1. Olde Hartman TC,
    2. Hassink-Franke LJ,
    3. Lucassen PL,
    4. et al.
    (2009) Explanation and relations. How do general practitioners deal with patients with persistent medically unexplained symptoms: a focus group study. BMC Fam Pract 10:68.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Nettleton S,
    2. Watt I,
    3. O’Malley L,
    4. Duffey P
    (2005) Understanding the narratives of people who live with medically unexplained illness. Patient Educ Couns 56(2):205–210.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    1. Peters S,
    2. Rogers A,
    3. Salmon P,
    4. et al.
    (2009) What do patients choose to tell their doctors? Qualitative analysis of potential barriers to reattributing medically unexplained symptoms. J Gen Intern Med 24(4):443–449.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. 10.↵
    1. Houwen J,
    2. Lucassen PLBJ,
    3. Stappers HW,
    4. et al.
    (2017) Medically unexplained symptoms: the person, the symptoms and the dialogue. Fam Pract 34(2):245–251.
    OpenUrl
  11. 11.↵
    1. Olde Hartman TC,
    2. Blankenstein AH,
    3. Molenaar AO,
    4. et al.
    (2013) NHG-Standaard Somatisch Onvoldoende verklaarde Lichamelijk Klachten (SOLK). [NHG guideline on medically unexplained symptoms (MUS)]. Huisarts en Wetenschap 56(5):222–230.
    OpenUrl
  12. 12.↵
    1. Rief W,
    2. Broadbent E
    (2007) Explaining medically unexplained symptoms — models and mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev 27(7):821–841.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    1. Fink P,
    2. Rosendal M,
    3. Toft T
    (2002) Assessment and treatment of functional disorders in general practice: the extended reattribution and management model — an advanced educational program for nonpsychiatric doctors. Psychosomatics 43(2):93–131.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. 14.↵
    1. Burton C,
    2. Weller D,
    3. Marsden W,
    4. et al.
    (2012) A primary care symptoms clinic for patients with medically unexplained symptoms: pilot randomised trial. BMJ Open 2:e000513.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. 15.↵
    1. Joosten A,
    2. Mazeland H,
    3. Meyboom-de Jong B
    (1999) Psychosocial explanations of complaints in Dutch general practice. Fam Pract 16(3):245–249.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    1. May C,
    2. Allison G,
    3. Chapple A,
    4. et al.
    (2004) Framing the doctor–patient relationship in chronic illness: a comparative study of general practitioners’ accounts. Sociol Health Illn 26(2):135–158.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. 17.↵
    1. Banks J,
    2. Prior L
    (2001) Doing things with illness. The micro politics of the CFS clinic. Soc Sci Med 52(1):11–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Burbaum C,
    2. Stresing AM,
    3. Fritzsche K,
    4. et al.
    (2010) Medically unexplained symptoms as a threat to patients’ identity? A conversation analysis of patients’ reactions to psychosomatic attributions. Patient Educ Couns 79(2):207–217.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. 19.↵
    1. Monzoni CM,
    2. Duncan R,
    3. Grünewald R,
    4. Reuber M
    (2011) Are there interactional reasons why doctors may find it hard to tell patients that their physical symptoms may have emotional causes? A conversation analytic study in neurology outpatients. Patient Educ Couns 85(3):e189–e200.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  20. 20.↵
    1. Feinstein R,
    2. Connelly J,
    3. Feinstein M
    1. Landa A,
    2. Makous M,
    3. Fallon BA
    (2017) in Integrating behavioral health and primary care, Treating somatic symptom disorder and illness anxiety in integrated care settings, eds Feinstein R, Connelly J, Feinstein M (Oxford University Press, Oxford), pp 266–302.
  21. 21.
    1. Gask L,
    2. Dowrick C,
    3. Salmon P,
    4. et al.
    (2011) Reattribution reconsidered: narrative review and reflections on an educational intervention for medically unexplained symptoms in primary care settings. J Psychosom Res 71(5):325–334.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. 22.↵
    1. Blankenstein AH,
    2. van der Horst HE,
    3. Schilte AF,
    4. et al.
    (2002) Development and feasibility of a modified reattribution model for somatising patients, applied by their own general practitioners. Patient Educ Couns 47(3):229–235.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. 23.↵
    1. Morton L,
    2. Elliott A,
    3. Thomas R,
    4. et al.
    (2016) Developmental study of treatment fidelity, safety and acceptability of a symptoms clinic intervention delivered by general practitioners to patients with multiple medically unexplained symptoms. J Psychosom Res 84:37–43.
    OpenUrl
  24. 24.↵
    1. Kroenke K,
    2. Spitzer RL,
    3. Williams JB,
    4. Lowe B
    (2010) The Patient Health Questionnaire Somatic, Anxiety, and Depressive Symptom Scales: a systematic review. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 32(4):345–359.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Spitzer RL,
    2. Kroenke K,
    3. Williams JB,
    4. Lowe B
    (2006) A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med 166(10):1092–1097.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    1. Zimmermann C,
    2. Del Piccolo L,
    3. Finset A
    (2007) Cues and concerns by patients in medical consultations: a literature review. Psychol Bull 133(3):438–463.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. Stone AL,
    2. Tai-Seale M,
    3. Stults CD,
    4. et al.
    (2012) Three types of ambiguity in coding empathic interactions in primary care visits: implications for research and practice. Patient Educ Couns 89(1):63–68.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  28. 28.↵
    1. Hsieh HF,
    2. Shannon SE
    (2005) Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qual Health Res 15(9):1277–1288.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Graneheim UH,
    2. Lundman B
    (2004) Qualitative content analysis in nursing research: concepts, procedures and measures to achieve trustworthiness. Nurse Educ Today 24(2):105–112.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    1. Ziebland S,
    2. McPherson A
    (2006) Making sense of qualitative data analysis: an introduction with illustrations from DIPEx (personal experiences of health and illness). Med Educ 40(5):405–414.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    1. Sidnell J
    (2010) Conversation analysis: an introduction (Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford).
  32. 32.↵
    1. Salmon P,
    2. Dowrick CF,
    3. Ring A,
    4. Humphris GM
    (2004) Voiced but unheard agendas: qualitative analysis of the psychosocial cues that patients with unexplained symptoms present to general practitioners. Br J Gen Pract 54(500):171–176.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  33. 33.
    1. Eide H,
    2. Sibbern T,
    3. Egeland T,
    4. et al.
    (2011) Fibromyalgia patients’ communication of cues and concerns: interaction analysis of pain clinic consultations. Clin J Pain 27(7):602–610.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  34. 34.
    1. Aiarzaguena JM,
    2. Gaminde I,
    3. Clemente I,
    4. Garrido E
    (2013) Explaining medically unexplained symptoms: somatizing patients’ responses in primary care. Patient Educ Couns 93(1):63–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Burton C,
    2. McGorm K,
    3. Weller D,
    4. Sharpe M
    (2011) The interpretation of low mood and worry by high users of secondary care with medically unexplained symptoms. BMC Fam Pract 12:107.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  36. 36.↵
    1. Chevalier F,
    2. Moore J
    1. Monzoni CM,
    2. Reuber M
    (2015) in Constraints and interactional restrictions in institutional talk: studies in conversation analysis, Linguistic and interactional restrictions in an outpatient clinic The challenge of delivering the diagnosis and explaining the aetiology of functional neurological problems, eds Chevalier F, Moore J (John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam), pp 239–270.
  37. 37.↵
    1. Goldberg D,
    2. Gask L,
    3. O’Dowd T
    (1989) The treatment of somatization: teaching techniques of reattribution. J Psychosom Res 33(6):689–695.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. 38.↵
    1. Morriss R,
    2. Dowrick C,
    3. Salmon P,
    4. et al.
    (2007) Cluster randomised controlled trial of training practices in reattribution for medically unexplained symptoms. Br J Psychiatry 191:536–542.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. 39.↵
    1. Morriss R,
    2. Gask L,
    3. Dowrick C,
    4. et al.
    (2010) Randomized trial of reattribution on psychosocial talk between doctors and patients with medically unexplained symptoms. Psychol Med 40(2):325–333.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. 40.↵
    1. Burton CD
    (2013) The ABC of medically unexplained symptoms (Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford).
  41. 41.↵
    1. Henningsen P
    (2018) Management of somatic symptom disorder. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 20(1):23–31.
    OpenUrl
  42. 42.↵
    1. Terluin B,
    2. van Marwijk HW,
    3. Adèr HJ,
    4. et al.
    (2006) The Four-Dimensional Symptom Questionnaire (4DSQ): a validation study of a multidimensional self-report questionnaire to assess distress, depression, anxiety and somatization. BMC Psychiatry 6:34.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. 43.↵
    1. Donner-Banzhoff N,
    2. Hertwig R
    (2014) Inductive foraging: improving the diagnostic yield of primary care consultations. Eur J Gen Pract 20(1):69–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. 44.↵
    1. van Ravesteijn HJ,
    2. Lucassen PL,
    3. olde Hartman TC
    (2008) Reattribution for medically unexplained symptoms. Br J Psychiatry 192(4):314–315, author reply 315.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
Back to top
Previous ArticleNext Article

In this issue

British Journal of General Practice: 70 (691)
British Journal of General Practice
Vol. 70, Issue 691
February 2020
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Download PDF
Email Article

Thank you for recommending British Journal of General Practice.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person to whom you are recommending the page knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Patients’ descriptions of the relation between physical symptoms and negative emotions: a qualitative analysis of primary care consultations
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from British Journal of General Practice
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from British Journal of General Practice.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Patients’ descriptions of the relation between physical symptoms and negative emotions: a qualitative analysis of primary care consultations
Ella Bekhuis, Janna Gol, Christopher Burton, Judith Rosmalen
British Journal of General Practice 2020; 70 (691): e78-e85. DOI: 10.3399/bjgp19X707369

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Share
Patients’ descriptions of the relation between physical symptoms and negative emotions: a qualitative analysis of primary care consultations
Ella Bekhuis, Janna Gol, Christopher Burton, Judith Rosmalen
British Journal of General Practice 2020; 70 (691): e78-e85. DOI: 10.3399/bjgp19X707369
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
  • Mendeley logo Mendeley

Jump to section

  • Top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • INTRODUCTION
    • METHOD
    • RESULTS
    • DISCUSSION
    • Acknowledgments
    • Notes
    • REFERENCES
  • Figures & Data
  • Info
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Keywords

  • communication
  • consultation
  • emotions
  • medically unexplained symptoms
  • relation

More in this TOC Section

  • The impact of remote care approaches on continuity in primary care: a mixed-studies systematic review
  • Performance of ethnic minority versus White doctors in the MRCGP assessment 2016–2021: a cross-sectional study
  • Trends in the registration of anxiety in Belgian primary care from 2000 to 2021: a registry-based study
Show more Research

Related Articles

Cited By...

Intended for Healthcare Professionals

BJGP Life

BJGP Open

 

@BJGPjournal's Likes on Twitter

 
 

British Journal of General Practice

NAVIGATE

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • All Issues
  • Online First
  • Authors & reviewers

RCGP

  • BJGP for RCGP members
  • BJGP Open
  • RCGP eLearning
  • InnovAiT Journal
  • Jobs and careers

MY ACCOUNT

  • RCGP members' login
  • Subscriber login
  • Activate subscription
  • Terms and conditions

NEWS AND UPDATES

  • About BJGP
  • Alerts
  • RSS feeds
  • Facebook
  • Twitter

AUTHORS & REVIEWERS

  • Submit an article
  • Writing for BJGP: research
  • Writing for BJGP: other sections
  • BJGP editorial process & policies
  • BJGP ethical guidelines
  • Peer review for BJGP

CUSTOMER SERVICES

  • Advertising
  • Contact subscription agent
  • Copyright
  • Librarian information

CONTRIBUTE

  • BJGP Life
  • eLetters
  • Feedback

CONTACT US

BJGP Journal Office
RCGP
30 Euston Square
London NW1 2FB
Tel: +44 (0)20 3188 7400
Email: journal@rcgp.org.uk

British Journal of General Practice is an editorially-independent publication of the Royal College of General Practitioners
© 2023 British Journal of General Practice

Print ISSN: 0960-1643
Online ISSN: 1478-5242